Skip to main content
Log in

Faux pas recognition performance in a help-seeking population at clinical high risk of psychosis

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

There is a growing body of evidence suggesting that patients with psychosis show impaired theory of mind (ToM). However, much remains to be understood as to whether ToM deficits occur in the premorbid or post-onset period of psychosis. Our primary aim was to examine empirically impairment on ToM tasks in a group of individuals with clinical high risk (CHR) of psychosis. Fifty CHR participants identified through the Structured Interview for Prodromal Syndromes and 52 age-/education-matched controls were assessed with a complete standard neuropsychological battery (the MCCB, MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery) and a social cognition assessment (Faux Pas Test, FPT). We then examined the association of baseline FPT performance with conversion to psychosis at 12-month follow-up. Compared with controls, the CHR group showed significantly poorer performances on the FPT and most MCCB domains. Significant positive correlations were found between faux pas detection and the MCCB domains of Attention/Vigilance and Working Memory in CHR participants when controlling for age and years of education. Mean scores on the FPT in 14 converters who were diagnosed with full-blown psychosis within 12 months were significantly lower than they were for non-converters. Impairments in ToM ability are acquired earlier in the prodromal stage of psychosis, along with general cognition (such as memory function) deficits. Declines in ToM ability may overlap with the progress of psychosis (the gradual loss insight), sharing similar neural substrates, and reflected by impairments in basic cognitive function.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Addington J, Penn D, Woods SW, Addington D, Perkins DO (2008) Facial affect recognition in individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis. Br J Psychiatry 192:67–68

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Ahmed FS, Stephen Miller L (2011) Executive function mechanisms of theory of mind. J Autism Dev Disord 41:667–678

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Apperly IA, Samson D, Humphreys GW (2009) Studies of adults can inform accounts of theory of mind development. Dev Psychol 45:190–201

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Baron-Cohen S, O’Riordan M, Stone V, Jones R, Plaisted K (1999) Recognition of faux pas by normally developing children and children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Autism Dev Disord 29:407–418

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Brune M (2003) Theory of mind and the role of IQ in chronic disorganized schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 60:57–64

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Burton CZ, Vella L, Harvey PD, Patterson TL, Heaton RK, Twamley EW (2013) Factor structure of the matrics consensus cognitive battery (MCCB) in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 146:244–248

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Carlson SM, Mandell DJ, Williams L (2004) Executive function and theory of mind: stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3. Dev Psychol 40:1105–1122

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Carlson SM, Moses LJ, Claxton LJ (2004) Individual differences in executive functioning and theory of mind: an investigation of inhibitory control and planning ability. J Exp Child Psychol 87:299–319

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Chung YS, Kang DH, Shin NY, Yoo SY, Kwon JS (2008) Deficit of theory of mind in individuals at ultra-high-risk for schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 99:111–118

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Couture SM, Granholm EL, Fish SC (2011) A path model investigation of neurocognition, theory of mind, social competence, negative symptoms and real-world functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 125:152–160

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Craig JS, Hatton C, Craig FB, Bentall RP (2004) Persecutory beliefs, attributions and theory of mind: comparison of patients with paranoid delusions, Asperger’s syndrome and healthy controls. Schizophr Res 69:29–33

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. David AS (1990) Insight and psychosis. Br J Psychiatry 156:798–808

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. De Herdt A, Wampers M, Vancampfort D, De Hert M, Vanhees L, Demunter H, Van Bouwel L, Brunner E, Probst M (2013) Neurocognition in clinical high risk young adults who did or did not convert to a first schizophrenic psychosis: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res 149:48–55

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Eack SM, Pogue-Geile MF, Greeno CG, Keshavan MS (2009) Evidence of factorial variance of the Mayer–Salovey–Caruso emotional intelligence test across schizophrenia and normative samples. Schizophr Res 114:105–109

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Fett AK, Viechtbauer W, Dominguez MD, Penn DL, van Os J, Krabbendam L (2011) The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 35:573–588

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Fine C, Lumsden J, Blair RJ (2001) Dissociation between ‘theory of mind’ and executive functions in a patient with early left amygdala damage. Brain 124:287–298

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Frith CD, Corcoran R (1996) Exploring ‘theory of mind’ in people with schizophrenia. Psychol Med 26:521–530

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Fusar-Poli P, Byrne M, Badger S, Valmaggia LR, McGuire PK (2013) Outreach and support in South London (OASIS), 2001–2011: ten years of early diagnosis and treatment for young individuals at high clinical risk for psychosis. Eur Psychiatry 28:315–326

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Giuliano AJ, Li H, Mesholam-Gately RI, Sorenson SM, Woodberry KA, Seidman LJ (2012) Neurocognition in the psychosis risk syndrome: a quantitative and qualitative review. Curr Pharm Des 18:399–415

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Gregory C, Lough S, Stone V, Erzinclioglu S, Martin L, Baron-Cohen S, Hodges JR (2002) Theory of mind in patients with frontal variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: theoretical and practical implications. Brain 125:752–764

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Happe FG (1994) An advanced test of theory of mind: understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. J Autism Dev Disord 24:129–154

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Harrington L, Siegert RJ, McClure J (2005) Theory of mind in schizophrenia: a critical review. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 10:249–286

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Hur JW, Byun MS, Shin NY, Shin YS, Kim SN, Jang JH, Kwon JS (2013) General intellectual functioning as a buffer against theory-of-mind deficits in individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis. Schizophr Res 149:83–87

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Kern RS, Nuechterlein KH, Green MF, Baade LE, Fenton WS, Gold JM, Keefe RS, Mesholam-Gately R, Mintz J, Seidman LJ, Stover E, Marder SR (2008) The matrics consensus cognitive battery, part 2: co-norming and standardization. Am J Psychiatry 165:214–220

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Kerr SL, Neale JM (1993) Emotion perception in schizophrenia: specific deficit or further evidence of generalized poor performance? J Abnorm Psychol 102:312–318

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Konstantakopoulos G, Ploumpidis D, Oulis P, Patrikelis P, Nikitopoulou S, Papadimitriou GN, David AS (2014) The relationship between insight and theory of mind in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 152:217–222

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Lancaster RS, Evans JD, Bond GR, Lysaker PH (2003) Social cognition and neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia. J Nerv Ment Dis 191:295–299

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Lee TY, Hong SB, Shin NY, Kwon JS (2015) Social cognitive functioning in prodromal psychosis: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res 164:28–34

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Lind SE, Williams DM, Raber J, Peel A, Bowler DM (2013) Spatial navigation impairments among intellectually high-functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder: exploring relations with theory of mind, episodic memory, and episodic future thinking. J Abnorm Psychol 122:1189–1199

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. McKinnon MC, Moscovitch M (2007) Domain-general contributions to social reasoning: theory of mind and deontic reasoning re-explored. Cognition 102:179–218

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Miller TJ, McGlashan TH, Rosen JL, Cadenhead K, Cannon T, Ventura J, McFarlane W, Perkins DO, Pearlson GD, Woods SW (2003) Prodromal assessment with the structured interview for prodromal syndromes and the scale of prodromal symptoms: predictive validity, interrater reliability, and training to reliability. Schizophr Bull 29:703–715

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Nuechterlein KH, Green MF, Kern RS, Baade LE, Barch DM, Cohen JD, Essock S, Fenton WS, Frese FJ 3rd, Gold JM, Goldberg T, Heaton RK, Keefe RS, Kraemer H, Mesholam-Gately R, Seidman LJ, Stover E, Weinberger DR, Young AS, Zalcman S, Marder SR (2008) The matrics consensus cognitive battery, part 1: test selection, reliability, and validity. Am J Psychiatry 165:203–213

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Peron J, Vicente S, Leray E, Drapier S, Drapier D, Cohen R, Biseul I, Rouaud T, Le Jeune F, Sauleau P, Verin M (2009) Are dopaminergic pathways involved in theory of mind? A study in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 47:406–414

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Pinkham AE, Penn DL, Perkins DO, Graham KA, Siegel M (2007) Emotion perception and social skill over the course of psychosis: a comparison of individuals “at-risk” for psychosis and individuals with early and chronic schizophrenia spectrum illness. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 12:198–212

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Prevost M, Carrier ME, Chowne G, Zelkowitz P, Joseph L, Gold I (2014) The reading the mind in the eyes test: validation of a French version and exploration of cultural variations in a multi-ethnic city. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 19:189–204

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Shahaeian A, Peterson CC, Slaughter V, Wellman HM (2011) Culture and the sequence of steps in theory of mind development. Dev Psychol 47:1239–1247

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, Hergueta T, Baker R, Dunbar GC (1998) The mini-international neuropsychiatric interview (MINI): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry Suppl 59(20):22–33 (quiz 34–57)

    Google Scholar 

  38. Shi C, He Y, Cheung EF, Yu X, Chan RC (2013) An ecologically valid performance-based social functioning assessment battery for schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 210:787–793

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Sprong M, Schothorst P, Vos E, Hox J, van Engeland H (2007) Theory of mind in schizophrenia: meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry 191:5–13

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Stanford AD, Messinger J, Malaspina D, Corcoran CM (2011) Theory of mind in patients at clinical high risk for psychosis. Schizophr Res 131:11–17

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Stone VE, Baron-Cohen S, Calder A, Keane J, Young A (2003) Acquired theory of mind impairments in individuals with bilateral amygdala lesions. Neuropsychologia 41:209–220

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Stone VE, Baron-Cohen S, Knight RT (1998) Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. J Cogn Neurosci 10:640–656

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Sullivan HS (1994) The onset of schizophrenia. 1927. Am J Psychiatry 151:134–139

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Thompson AD, Bartholomeusz C, Yung AR (2011) Social cognition deficits and the ‘ultra high risk’ for psychosis population: a review of literature. Early Interv Psychiatry 5:192–202

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Vauth R, Rusch N, Wirtz M, Corrigan PW (2004) Does social cognition influence the relation between neurocognitive deficits and vocational functioning in schizophrenia? Psychiatry Res 128:155–165

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Wimmer H, Perner J (1983) Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition 13:103–128

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Wood SJ, Brewer WJ, Koutsouradis P, Phillips LJ, Francey SM, Proffitt TM, Yung AR, Jackson HJ, McGorry PD, Pantelis C (2007) Cognitive decline following psychosis onset: data from the pace clinic. Br J Psychiatry Suppl 51:s52–s57. doi:10.1192/bjp.191.51.s52

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Woodberry KA, Giuliano AJ, Seidman LJ (2008) Premorbid IQ in schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Am J Psychiatry 165:579–587

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Yong E, Barbato M, Penn DL, Keefe RS, Woods SW, Perkins DO, Addington J (2014) Exploratory analysis of social cognition and neurocognition in individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis. Psychiatry Res 218:39–43

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Zhang T, Li H, Woodberry KA, Seidman LJ, Zheng L, Zhao S, Tang Y, Guo Q, Lu X, Zhuo K, Qian Z, Chow A, Li C, Jiang K, Xiao Z, Wang J (2014) Prodromal psychosis detection in a counseling center population in China: an epidemiological and clinical study. Schizophr Res 152:391–399

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Zheng L, Wang J, Zhang T, Li H, Li C, Jiang K (2012) The Chinese version of the SIPS/SOPS: a pilot study of reliability and validity. Chin Ment Health J 26:571–576

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by National Natural Science Foundation of China (81201043, 81171267, 81171280, 61102020, 81261120410, 81361120403), Shanghai Municipal Natural Science Foundation (12ZR1448400), Shanghai Science and Technology Committee (15411967200), National Key Clinical Disciplines at Shanghai Mental Health Center (Office of Medical Affairs, Ministry of Health, 2011-873; OMA-MH, 2011-873), Shanghai Clinical Center for Mental Disorders(2014), Shanghai Key Laboratory of Psychotic Disorders (13dz2260500), Doctoral Innovation Fund Projects from Shanghai Jiao Tong University School of Medicine (BXJ 201345), Shanghai Jiao Tong University Foundation (14JCRY04, YG2014MS40), and by a Fogarty and National Institutes of Mental Health Grant (1R21 MH093294-01A1) from the USA.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to ZePing Xiao or JiJun Wang.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

None.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Zhang, T., Yi, Z., Li, H. et al. Faux pas recognition performance in a help-seeking population at clinical high risk of psychosis. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 266, 71–78 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-015-0615-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-015-0615-z

Keywords

Navigation