Skip to main content
Log in

Not so Simple Addition: Comparing Student Performance and Teacher Perceptions of Retrieval

  • Published:
International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

There has been considerable research into how children solve single-digit addition problems for more than a century now, which has brought about significant changes to the ways teachers’ support children to retrieve single-digit addition facts. In this project, we examine what makes an addition problem difficult to retrieve in light of contemporary teaching approaches, based on both student performance and teacher perceptions. Australian primary school students in Years 3 and 4 (n = 166) solved 36 single-digit addition problems under two different conditions during a structured interview. These data were then used to create the Difficulty Retrieving Addition Facts (DRAF) measure, which we propose as a contemporary measure of single-digit addition problem difficulty to supersede older measures developed in a different era of instruction. We then invited Australian primary school teachers (n = 49) to complete a questionnaire asking them to estimate the percentage of students who would be able to rapidly retrieve these same 36 single-digit addition problems to facilitate comparison between student performance and teacher perceptions. We found that although teachers were generally accurate in discerning which addition problems students would find relatively easy to retrieve and which they would find more difficult, they tended to overestimate student capacity to retrieve addition facts in general, particularly when the addition fact was comparatively difficult. We suggest that this overestimation resulted from teacher responses being shaped by curriculum expectations, which states that students should be able to recall single-digit addition facts by the end of Year 3.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Note that the terms recall and retrieve are used interchangeably throughout this paper. When referring to the notion of “just knowing” the answer to a single-digit addition problem, whilst the Australian Curriculum uses the term “recall,” the research literature tends to use the term “retrieve” (e.g. Campbell & Oliphant, 1992).

  2. This was achieved by subtracting the minimum score on the composite scale from each of the item scores on the composite scale.

  3. The reason that the correlations between DRAF score and the various measures of problem size are lower than Wheeler’s (1939) study when doubles facts are included is because our study did not include “add zero,” “add one,” or “reciprocal” problems (e.g. we did not include both 2 + 9 and 9 + 2, only the former). The inclusion of these additional problems would have had the effect of strengthening these correlations.

References

  • Ashcraft, M. H. (1987). Children’s knowledge of simple arithmetic: A developmental model and simulation. In J. Bisanz, C. J. Brainerd, & R. Kail (Eds.), Formal methods in developmental psychology: Progress in cognitive development research (pp. 302–338). SpringerVerlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ashcraft, M. H., & Christy, K. S. (1995). The frequency of arithmetic facts in elementary texts: Addition and multiplication in grades 1–6. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 26(5), 396–421. https://doi.org/10.2307/749430

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Australian Curriculum and Assessment Authority (ACARA). (2015). The Australian curriculum: Mathematics. Retrieved from http://www.australiancurriculum.edu.au/mathematics/curriculum/f-10?layout=1. Accessed 1 Feb 2022

  • Australian Curriculum and Assessment Authority (ACARA). (2021). National assessment program—literacy and numeracy achievement in reading, writing, language conventions and numeracy: National report for 2021. Retrieved from https://www.nap.edu.au/results-and-reports/national-reports. Accessed 1 Feb 2022

  • Baroody, A. J. (1994). An evaluation of evidence supporting fact-retrieval models. Learning and Individual Differences, 6(1), 1–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/1041-6080(94)90013-2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baroody, A. J. (2006). Why children have difficulties mastering the basic number combinations and how to help them. Teaching Children Mathematics, 13(1), 22–31. https://doi.org/10.5951/tcm.13.1.0022

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Browne, C. E. (1906). The psychology of simple arithmetical processes: A study of certain habits of attention and association. American Journal of Psychology, 17, 1–37. https://doi.org/10.2307/1412457

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brownell, W. A. (1928). The development of children’s number ideas in the primary grades (Supplementary Education Monograph No. 35). University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, B., Zorzi, M., Girelli, L., & Jonckheere, A. R. (2001). Storage and retrieval of addition facts: The role of number comparison. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 54, 1005–1029. https://doi.org/10.1080/713756007

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, J. I. D., & Oliphant, M. (1992). Representation and retrieval of arithmetic facts: A network-interference model and simulation. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), The nature and origin of mathematical skills (pp. 331–364). Elsevier Science. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0166-4115(08)60891-2

  • Campbell, J. I. D., & Xue, Q. (2001). Cognitive arithmetic across cultures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 299–315. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.130.2.299

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, T. P., Fennema, E., Franke, M. L., Levi, L., & Empson, S. B. (2015). Children’s mathematics: Cognitively guided instruction (2nd ed.). Heinemann

  • Christensen, C. A., & Cooper, T. J. (1991). Children’s solution strategies to single-digit addition problems: Some Australian data. Education Research and Perspectives, 18(2), 62–70. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532690xci0804_6

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2014). Learning and teaching early math. The learning trajectories approach. Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, R. (2003). Does it all add up? Changes in children’s knowledge of addition combinations, strategies, and principles. In A.J. Baroody and A. Dowker (Eds.), The development of arithmetic concepts and skills: Constructive adaptive expertise (pp. 35–74). Taylor and Francis Group. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781410607218-7

  • Didino, D. (2011). A study on the representation of the arithmetic facts memory: Cognitively speaking, is the commutativity a property of multiplications and additions? (Doctoral dissertation). University of Trento.

  • Fuson, K. C., & Kwon, Y. (1992). Korean children’s single-digit addition and subtraction: Numbers structured by ten. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 23, 148–165. https://doi.org/10.2307/749498

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gervasoni, A., Guimelli, K., & McHugh, B. (2017). The development of addition and subtraction strategies for children in kindergarten to grade 6: Insights and implications. In A. Downton, S. Livy, & J. Hall (Eds.), 40 years on: We are still learning! Proceedings of the 40th annual conference of the Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia (pp. 269–276). MERGA.

  • Hopkins, S., & Bayliss, D. (2017). The prevalence and disadvantage of min-counting in seventh grade: Problems with confidence as well as accuracy? Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 19(1), 19–32. https://doi.org/10.1080/10986065.2017.1258613

  • Hopkins, S., & de Villiers, C. (2016). Improving the efficiency of problem-solving practice for children with retrieval difficulties. In B. White, M. Chinnappan, & S. Trenholm (Eds.), Proceedings of the 39th Annual Conference of the Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia (pp. 311–318). Adelaide: MERGA.

  • LeFevre, J. A., Sadesky, G. S., & Bisanz, J. (1996). Selection of procedures in mental addition: Reassessing the problem size effect in adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(1), 216–230. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.22.1.216

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MySchool. (2017). MySchool Website, from www.myschool.edu.au. Accessed 1 Feb 2022

  • Ostermann, A., Leuders, T., & Nückles, M. (2018). Improving the judgment of task difficulties: Prospective teachers’ diagnostic competence in the area of functions and graphs. Journal of Mathematics Teacher Education, 21(6), 579–605. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10857-017-9369-z

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramani, G. B., & Siegler, R. S. (2008). Promoting broad and stable improvements in low-income children’s numerical knowledge through playing number board games. Child Development, 79(2), 375–394. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01131.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shrager, J., & Siegler, R. S. (1998). SCADS: A model of children’s strategy choices and strategy discoveries. Psychological Science, 9(5), 405–410. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00076

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. (1987). The perils of averaging data over strategies: An example from children’s addition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116(3), 250–264. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.116.3.250

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S., & Robinson, M. (1982). The development of numerical understandings. In H. W. Reese & L. P. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in child development and behaviour (Vol. 16, pp. 241–312). JAI. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0065-2407(08)60072-5

  • Südkamp, A., Kaiser, J., & Möller, J. (2012). Accuracy of teachers’ judgments of students’ academic achievement: A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(3), 743–762.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Urhahne, D., & Wijnia, L. (2021). A review on the accuracy of teacher judgments. Educational Research Review, 32, 100374. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2020.100374

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, M. (Ed.). (2008). Children learn mathematics: A learning-teaching trajectory with intermediate attainment targets for calculation with whole numbers in primary school. Sense Publishers. https://doi.org/10.1163/9789087903954

  • Wheeler, L. R. (1939). A comparative study of the difficulty of the 100 addition combinations. The Pedagogical Seminary and Journal of Genetic Psychology, 54(2), 295–312. https://doi.org/10.1080/08856559.1939.10534337

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Widaman, K. F., Geary, D. C., Cormier, P., & Little, T. D. (1989). A componential model for mental addition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(5), 898–919. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.15.5.898

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yuan, C., & Yang, H. (2019). Research on K-value selection method of K-means clustering algorithm. J—Multidisciplinary Scientific Journal, 2(2), 226–235. https://doi.org/10.3390/j2020016

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zbrodoff, N. J., & Logan, G. D. (2005). What everyone finds: The problem–size effect. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of mathematical cognition (pp. 331–345). Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203998045-29

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to James Russo.

Ethics declarations

Ethics Approval

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board (or Ethics Committee) of Monash University.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Russo, J., Hopkins, S. Not so Simple Addition: Comparing Student Performance and Teacher Perceptions of Retrieval. Int J of Sci and Math Educ 21, 2279–2301 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10763-022-10346-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10763-022-10346-7

Keywords

Navigation