Skip to main content
Log in

Grammaticality, acceptability, possible words and large corpora

  • Published:
Morphology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The related notions of possible word, actual word and productivity are difficult to work with because of the difficulty with the notion of actual word. When large corpora are used as a source of data, there are some benefits for the practising morphologist, but the notion of actual word becomes even more difficult. This is because it rapidly becomes clear that in corpora there may be more than one form for the same morphosemantic complex, so that rules may have multiple outputs. One of the factors that may help determine the output of a variable rule in morphology is the productivity of the process involved. If that is the case, the notion of productivity has to be reevaluated.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. An earlier version of this paper was presented at the conference on Data-Rich Approaches to English Morphology, held in Wellington, New Zealand, in July 2012. I should like to thank attendees at the conference, Liza Tarasova and Natalia Beliaeva, and referees for Morphology for feedback on earlier versions, and Jonathan Newton for the example in (2). The research for this paper was supported by a grant from the Royal Society of New Zealand through its Marsden Fund to the author.

  2. In the case of conversion I assume that this still applies, in that the ‘simpler word’ has not undergone the identity operation which creates the derivative. Clearly, an alternative position would be possible, in which case the characterization of a rule given here would have to be modified slightly.

  3. OED. The Oxford English dictionary on-line. www.oed.com.

  4. Referees for Morphology rightly query the use of the term ‘infinite’ in this context, yet Chomsky (1975:78) talks of “an infinite set of grammatical utterances”, Fromkin et al. (1999:9) of “an infinite set of new sentences” and Carnie (2007:16) says “Language is a productive (probably infinite) system”, including in his reasoning that sentences can always be extended by the addition of an extra sub-part. Perhaps words cannot always be so extended. I find Matthews (1979:24) helpful: “A generativist says that the speaker’s mind controls an infinite set of sentences. But this is not a statement of observed fact. It is part of a theory.”

  5. And can be attested at http://qahatesyou.com/wordpress/category/philosophy/ (accessed 9 Jan 2013).

  6. Indeed, even the Japanese example below may not stand up to close scrutiny, since Miyagawa notes a slightly different function for the two causative markers, despite their shared form and shared meaning.

  7. There is some variability as to the resyllabification of the final <r> in English: manageress shows no resyllabification, and this is not simply a matter of whether we are dealing with -er or -or, since we find doctoress, mayoress and waitress. The -erer sequence can be found in forms like adulterer (where only one of the -er sequences is an independent morph) or in fruiterer, which is unusual in its structure, and not obviously productive.

  8. Unfuckingremarkable is apparently (Google) less remarkable than unrefuckingmarkable, despite claims in the literature that the placement of expletives in such words is largely prosodically determined.

  9. The distinction between e.g. deduct and deduce as corresponding to deduction (in the Sherlock Holmes sense of deduction, not the arithmetical one) is usually discussed as formation versus back-formation. This assumes a model where speakers always go from the morphologically simpler to the morphologically more complex when coining words. Speakers make a large number of what would normatively be called ‘errors’ in the stress patterns on verbs because they try to retain the stress pattern from a morphologically more complex derivative in the morphologically simpler verb. That is we hear forms like propagáte rather than the expected própagate, presumably influenced by propagátion. At the very least this calls into question the way in which real speakers operate.

  10. Baayen and Lieber have the frequency of new morphologically simplex words as the baseline, above which things count as productive, which is a much better justified level and rather more inclusive than this proposal, which is really just included to allow for the argument.

References

  • Albright, A. (2009). Modeling analogy as probabilistic grammar. In J. P. Blevins & J. Blevins (Eds.), Analogy in grammar: form and acquisition (pp. 185–204). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Aronoff, M. (1976). Word formation in generative grammar. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R. H. (2009). Corpus linguistics in morphology: morphological productivity. In A. Luedeling & M. Kytö (Eds.), Corpus linguistics: an international handbook (pp. 900–919). Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, H., & Lieber, R. (1991). Productivity and English derivation: a corpus-based study. Linguistics, 29, 801–843.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, L. (1983). English word-formation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, L. (1996). Is morphological productivity non-linguistic? Acta Linguistica Hungarica, 43, 19–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, L. (1998). When is a sequence of two nouns a compound in English? English Language and Linguistics, 2, 65–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, L. (2001). Morphological productivity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, L., Lieber, R., & Plag, I. (2013). The Oxford reference guide to English morphology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bender, E. M. (2006). Variation and formal theories of language: HPSG. In K. Brown (Ed.), Encyclopedia of language and linguistics (2nd ed., Vol. 13, pp. 326–329). Oxford: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Britain, D. (2000). As far as analysing grammatical variation and change in New Zealand English with relatively few tokens <is concerned/Ø>. In A. Bell & K. Kuiper (Eds.), Focus on New Zealand English (pp. 198–220). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Carnie, A. (2007). Syntax (2nd ed.). Malden: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carstairs-McCarthy, A. (1992). Current morphology. London: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. Paris: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1975). The logical structure of linguistic theory. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corbin, D. (1987). Morphologie dérivationelle et structuration du lexique (Vols. 1, 2). Tübingen: Niemeyer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Davies, M. (2008). The Corpus of Contemporary American English (COCA): 400+ million words. 1990-present. http://www.americancorpus.org/.

  • Di Sciullo, A. M., & Williams, E. (1987). On the definition of word. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dressler, W. U., Libben, G., Stark, J., Pons, C., & Jarema, G. (2001). The processing of interfixed German compounds. In: Yearbook of morphology 1999 (pp. 185–220).

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Fabb, N. (1988). English suffixation is constrained only by selectional restrictions. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 6, 527–539.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fleischer, W., & Barz, I. (2007). Wortbildung der deutschen Gegenwartssprache. Tübingen: Niemeyer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fromkin, V., Blair, D., & Collins, P. (1999). An introduction to language (4th Australian ed.). Sydney: Harcourt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halle, M. (1973). Prolegomena to a theory of word formation. Linguistic Inquiry, 4, 3–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, Z. S. (1951). Structural linguistics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hay, J. (2003). Causes and consequences of word structure. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hundt, M. (2013). Error, feature, or (incipient) change? In Englishes today conference, Vigo, 18–19 October 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff, R. (1975). Morphological and semantic regularities in the lexicon. Language, 51, 639–671.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, J. T. (1990). Morphology. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Jespersen, O. (1917). Negation in English and other languages. Copenhagen: Bianco Lunos Bogtrykkeri.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koefoed, G. A. T. (1992). Analogie is geen taalverandering. Forum der Letteren, 33, 11–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krott, A. (2001). Analogy in morphology. Doctoral dissertation, Katholieke Universiteit Nijmegen.

  • Krott, A., Schreuder, R., & Baayen, R. H. (2002). Linking elements in Dutch noun noun compounds: constituent families as analogical predictors for response latencies. Brain and Language, 81, 723–735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krott, A., Schreuder, R., Baayen, R. H., & Dressler, W. U. (2007). Analogical effects on linking elements in German compounds. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22, 25–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Labov, W. (1972). Sociolinguistic patterns. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lappe, S. (2007). English prosodic morphology. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ljung, M. (1970). English denominal adjectives. Lund: Acta Universitatis Gothoburgensis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyons, J. (1991). Chomsky (3rd ed.). London: Fontana.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marchand, H. (1969). The categories and types of present-day English word-formation (2nd ed.). Munich: Beck.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, P. H. (1979). Generative grammar and linguistic competence. London: Allen & Unwin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, D. G. (1993). Complex verb formation. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Miyagawa, S. (1999). Causatives. In N. Tsujimura (Ed.), The handbook of Japanese linguistics (pp. 236–268). Malden: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neef, M. (2009). IE, Germanic German. In R. Lieber & P. Stekauer (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of compounding (pp. 386–399). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Orgun, C. O., & Sprouse, R. L. (1999). From MParse to control: deriving ungrammaticality. Phonology, 16, 191–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Payne, J., & Huddleston, R. (2002). Nouns and noun phrases. In R. Huddleston & G. K. Pullum (Eds.), The Cambridge grammar of the English language (pp. 323–523). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pearsall, J. (Ed.) (2002). The concise Oxford dictionary (10th ed., revised). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plag, I. (1999). Morphological productivity. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Plag, I. (2003). Word-formation in English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Plag, I., Dalton-Puffer, C., & Baayen, H. (1999). Morphological productivity across speech and writing. English Language and Linguistics, 3, 209–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radford, A., Felser, C., & Boxell, O. (2012). Preposition copying and pruning in present-day English. English Language and Linguistics, 16, 403–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rainer, F. (1988). Towards a theory of blocking: the case of Italian and German quality nouns. In Yearbook of Morphology (pp. 155–185).

    Google Scholar 

  • Renouf, A. (2013). A finer definition of neology in English: the life-cycle of a word. In H. Hasselgård, J. Ebeling, & S. O. Ebeling (Eds.), Corpus perspectives on patterns of lexis (pp. 177–207). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Skousen, R. (1989). Analogical modeling of language. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • The British National Corpus, version 3 (BNC XML Edition) (2007). Distributed by Oxford University Computing Services on behalf of the BNC Consortium. http://www.natcorp.ox.ac.uk/.

  • Thornton, A. M. (2012). Reduction and maintenance of overabundance: a case study on Italian verb paradigms. Word Structure, 5, 183–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmer, K. E. (1964). Affixal negation in English and other languages: an investigation of restricted productivity. Word, 20, 21–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zwicky, A. M. (1969). Phonological constraints in syntactic descriptions. Papers in Linguistics, 1, 411–463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Laurie Bauer.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bauer, L. Grammaticality, acceptability, possible words and large corpora. Morphology 24, 83–103 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-014-9234-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-014-9234-z

Keywords

Navigation