Elsevier

Psychoneuroendocrinology

Volume 104, June 2019, Pages 49-54
Psychoneuroendocrinology

Prenatal testosterone exposure is associated with delay of gratification and attention problems/overactive behavior in 3-year-old boys

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.02.014Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Girls are better able to delay gratification than boys.

  • Boys with higher prenatal testosterone levels are less able to delay gratification.

  • Boys with higher prenatal testosterone levels show more attentions problems/overactive behavior.

  • Boys with more attention problems/overactive behavior are less able to delay gratification.

Abstract

Sex differences in self-control become apparent during preschool years. Girls are better able to delay their gratification and show less attention problems and overactive behavior than boys. In this context, organizational effects of gonadal steroids affecting the neural circuitry underlying self-control could be responsible for these early sex differences. In the present study testosterone levels measured in amniotic fluid (via ultra performance liquid chromatography and tandem mass spectrometry) were used to examine the role of organizational sex hormones on self-control. One hundred and twenty-two 40-month-old children participated in a delay of gratification task (DoG task) and their parents reported on their attention problems and overactive behavior. Girls waited significantly longer for their preferred reward than boys, and significantly more girls than boys waited the maximum period of time, providing evidence for sex differences in delay of gratification. Boys that were rated as suffering from more attention problems and overactive behavior waited significantly shorter in the DoG task. Amniotic testosterone measures were reliable in boys only. Most importantly, boys who waited shorter in the DoG task and boys who were reported to suffer from more attention problems and overactive behavior had higher prenatal testosterone levels. These findings extend our knowledge concerning organizational effects of testosterone on the brain circuitry underlying self-control in boys, and are of relevance for understanding how sex differences in behavioral disorders are connected with a lack of self-control.

Introduction

Delaying an immediate smaller reward for the sake of a later, larger one is incredibly difficult at all ages. Interestingly, the ability to show self-control by resisting predominant impulses for the pursuit of long-term goals already evolves early in life. Self-control in preschoolers is typically investigated with the delay of gratification (DoG) paradigm (Mischel and Ebbesen, 1970), which gives children the choice between an immediate smaller or less preferred reward (e.g. one marshmallow) and a later, larger or more preferred reward (e.g. two marshmallows). The relationship between delay time and parents’ as well as caregivers’ ratings of self-control corroborate the validity of the DoG paradigm as a measure of self-control (Duckworth et al., 2013).

A meta-analysis consisting of 33 studies confirmed a small, but significant, female advantage in DoG, which was substantially larger when waiting time was measured continuously instead of a dichotomous measurement of choice behavior (Silverman, 2003). In line with the female advantage in the DoG paradigm, 2135 parents reported better self-control in their 3- to 6-year-old daughters in comparison with their sons on a questionnaire (Tao et al., 2014).

These sex differences fit with the finding that substantially more boys than girls are diagnosed with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; Ramtekkar et al., 2010). Impulsive behavior (Duckworth and Steinberg, 2015) is one of the main DSM-5 (APA, 2013) diagnostic features of ADHD and is reflected by reward-delay impulsivity, defined as the choice of an immediate small reward over a delayed large reward (Patros et al., 2016). Consequently, children with ADHD differ from healthy children in their ability to delay gratification (for a meta-analysis see Patros et al., 2016). Additionally, parents’ estimates of attention deficits and hyperactive behavior on questionnaires correlate with the ability to delay gratification in samples of healthy children (Duckworth et al., 2013; Paloyelis et al., 2009). Sex differences in self-control also help to explain the gender gap in school grades. While girls achieve higher grades in the major subjects, they differ from boys in measures of self-control not IQ (Duckworth and Seligman, 2006; Duckworth et al., 2015; Matthews et al., 2009; Weis et al., 2013).

Many sex differences are affected by sex hormones. Testosterone especially, has organizational and activational effects. There is little and conflicting evidence concerning activational effects of testosterone on self-control in adults (Doi et al., 2015; Ortner et al., 2013; Peper et al., 2013). Since there are little variations in circulating sex hormones before puberty, prenatal (for a review see Cohen-Bendahan et al., 2005) or early postnatal testosterone exposure (for a review see Hines et al., 2016) having organizational effects on the brain is likely to play a role in the emergence of the early evident sexual dimorphism in self-control in childhood. Nevertheless, it is indisputable that self-control is affected by the social environment to a great extent. For example, parents serve as a model in the development of self-control in their children and influence their children’s self-control via caregiving behavior (for an overview see Bridgett et al., 2015). It also cannot be ruled out that boys and girls face different expectations regarding their self-control abilities (Bembenutty, 2007; Funder et al., 1983).

Supporting the hypothesis that self-control is influenced by prenatal testosterone, male rats normally make more impulsive choices than female rats in a delay-based impulsive choice task. However, these behavioral sex differences disappear when female rats are neonatally injected with testosterone (Bayless et al., 2013). In humans, there is only indirect evidence for prenatal testosterone effects on self-control from studies using the second-to-fourth-digit-ratio (2D:4D). 2D:4D is larger in females than in males early in life and is used as a marker for prenatal testosterone exposure (for a review see Breedlove, 2010). A study with preschoolers found that children who were less able to delay gratification had lower 2D:4D ratios (and supposedly higher prental testosterone levels; Da Silva et al., 2014). However, as this study did not apply separate analyses for boys and girls, the significant results might be due to the fact that both measures (2D:4D and delay of gratification) show sex differences. Another study with adults reported that women with smaller digit ratios on their right hands prefer smaller, more immediate over larger, delayed amounts of money (Lucas and Koff, 2010). Importantly, the specific relationship of 2D:4D with prenatal sex hormone levels is still under debate (Richards, 2017). Only one research group found the assumed negative relationship between 2D:4D and testosterone from amniotic fluid (a more direct measure) in girls (Ventura et al., 2013). Another report on a correlation between 2D:4D and the ratio between testosterone and estradiol from amniocentesis (Lutchmaya et al., 2004) did not separate the sexes for the correlational analyses. Clearly, evidence for an effect of prenatal steroids on self-control in children from a more direct measure of prenatal sex hormones, namely testosterone levels measured in amniotic fluid, is missing.

Amniocentesis is an invasive method of sampling amniotic fluid, normally conducted during the second trimester of pregnancy and is used only in cases of increased risk for genetic and chromosomal anomalies, like increased maternal age. Sex differences in testosterone from amniotic fluid emerge between week 8 and 24 of pregnancy peaking around week 17 (Judd et al., 1976). This corresponds quite well with the timing of amniocentesis and resembles the testosterone peak in serum (Finegan et al., 1989; although the exact relationship between amniotic and serum testosterone is still not completely understood - for a review see Constantinescu and Hines, 2012). Other sex dimorphic behaviors in early childhood like sex-typed play behavior and empathic behavior have already been found to be associated with prenatal testosterone levels from amniotic fluid (for a review see Auyeung et al., 2013).

The present study explores, for the first time, the effects of prenatal testosterone from amniotic fluid on self-control in early childhood. Self-control behavior was measured in 40-month-old children using a modified version of the DoG task (Mischel and Ebbesen, 1970). Attention problems/overactive behavior were obtained from the parents’ report in a questionnaire. Based on previous research, girls were expected to wait longer for their preferred reward than boys and boys to be reported to show more attention problems/overactive behavior than girls. According to the results from an animal study (Bayless et al., 2013) and 2D:4D studies in humans (Da Silva et al., 2014; Lucas and Koff, 2010), we expected children with higher amniotic testosterone levels to be less able to delay gratification in the DoG task and to show more attention problems/ overactive behavior. Furthermore, we expected children showing more attention problems/overactive behavior to be less able to delay gratification in the DoG task.

Section snippets

Participants

Between 2010 and 2012, a partnership of gynecologists and human geneticists in Duesseldorf, Germany, (Praenatal.de) recruited mothers who underwent amniocentesis for the present study. The children were born between January 2011 and February 2013.

One hundred and fifty-seven Caucasian children (78 female, 79 male) from Duesseldorf (Germany) and surrounding towns took part in the study at a mean age of 40.14 months (SD = 0.50 months). Sixteen boys and 18 girls had to be excluded from data

Results

Girls (M = 374.35 s; SD = 163.95 s) waited significantly longer than boys (M = 297.29 s; SD = 204.63 s; see Fig. 1) to receive their preferred reward (t(117.60) = 2.31, p =  .012, one-tailed, d = 0.42, unequal variances; Levene’s test of homogeneity of variances: F(1,121) = 13.70; p < .001) and more girls (63.3%) than boys (44.4%) delayed gratification for the maximum time span (8 min; χ²(1) = 4.41, p = .018, one-tailed, d = 0.39).

According to the parents’ answers on the PKBS-II Scale Attention

Discussion

The present research explores, for the first time, the role of prenatal sex hormones in early childhood self-control. As expected, girls outperformed boys in the ability to delay gratification. The girls waited longer than the boys to receive their preferred reward and more girls than boys waited the maximum time span. The most important finding of our study is the predicted relationship between self-control and amniotic testosterone in boys. Both self-control measures (the waiting time in the

Conclusions

This study is the first that shows a relationship between prenatal testosterone from amniocentesis and self-control for three-year-old boys. The findings extend our knowledge concerning organizational effects of testosterone on the brain circuitry underlying self-control and are of relevance for understanding how sex differences in behavioral disorders are connected with a lack of self-control.

Declarations of interest

None.

Funding

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

References (59)

  • M. Lucas et al.

    Delay discounting is associated with the 2D:4D ratio in women but not men

    Pers. Indiv. Differ.

    (2010)
  • S. Lutchmaya et al.

    2nd to 4th digit ratios, fetal testosterone and estradiol

    Early Hum. Dev.

    (2004)
  • G.R. Ortner et al.

    No evidence for an effect of testosterone administration on delay discounting in male university students

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2013)
  • Y. Paloyelis et al.

    Are ADHD symptoms associated with delay aversion or choice impulsivity? A general population study

    J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc.

    (2009)
  • V.L. Pasterski et al.

    Postnatal penile growth concurrent with mini-puberty predicts later sex-typed play behavior: evidence for neurobehavioral effects of the postnatal androgen surge in typically developing boys

    Horm. Behav.

    (2015)
  • C.H.G. Patros et al.

    Choice-impulsivity in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a meta-analytic review

    Clin. Psychol. Rev.

    (2016)
  • G. Richards

    What is the evidence for a link between digit ratio (2D:4D) and direct measures of prenatal sex hormones?

    Early Hum. Dev.

    (2017)
  • T. Ventura et al.

    Digit ratio (2D:4D) in newborns: influences of prenatal testosterone and maternal environment

    Early Hum. Dev.

    (2013)
  • J. Zhan et al.

    Spatial–temporal atlas of human fetal brain development during the early second trimester

    NeuroImage

    (2013)
  • APA

    Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders

    (2013)
  • B. Auyeung et al.

    Prenatal and postnatal hormone effects on the human brain and cognition

    Pflug Arch. Eur. J. Phys.

    (2013)
  • A. Al Awmleh et al.

    Reliability of the German language version of the preschool and kindergarten behavior scales second editon

    J. Soc. Sci.

    (2013)
  • H. Bembenutty

    Self-regulation of learning and academic delay of gratification: Gender and ethnic differences among college students

    J. Adv. Acad.

    (2007)
  • S.M. Breedlove

    Minireview: organizational hypothesis: instances of the fingerpost

    Endocrinology

    (2010)
  • D.J. Bridgett et al.

    Intergenerational transmission of self-regulation: a multidisciplinary review and integrative conceptual framework

    Psychol. Bull.

    (2015)
  • B.J. Casey

    Beyond simple models of self-control to circuit-based accounts of adolescent behavior

    Annu. Rev. Psychol.

    (2015)
  • A.S. Clark et al.

    Androgen binding and metabolism in the cerebral cortex of the developing rhesus monkey

    Endocrinology

    (1988)
  • J. Cohen

    Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences

    (1988)
  • M. Constantinescu et al.

    Relating prenatal testosterone exposure to postnatal behavior in typically developing children: methods and findings

    Child Dev. Perspect.

    (2012)
  • Cited by (14)

    • The effect of sex hormones on brain development and functioning

      2023, Encyclopedia of Child and Adolescent Health, First Edition
    • Prenatal urinary concentrations of phthalate metabolites and behavioral problems in Mexican children: The Programming Research in Obesity, Growth Environment and Social Stress (PROGRESS) study

      2021, Environmental Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Indeed, higher MONP concentrations were associated with reduced attention problems in boys and girls. DiNP, the parent compound of MONP, has been shown to reduce prenatal testosterone levels (Hannas et al., 2011), which may reduce attention problems (Körner et al., 2019). Several biological mechanisms may explain the associations between prenatal phthalate metabolite concentrations and childhood behavioral outcomes we observed in this study.

    • An examination of the influence of prenatal sex hormones on handedness: Literature review and amniotic fluid data

      2021, Hormones and Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      These cross-reactivities, or “specificities”, are usually reported by the manufacturer, though when using sample material for which the assay was not originally designed (e.g., amniotic fluid), this information may be inaccurate. Additionally, RIA is typically less precise than more modern techniques, such as liquid-chromatography tandem mass spectrometry (LC-MS) (e.g., Körner et al., 2019; Ventura et al., 2013), and it is not possible to accurately compare absolute values across studies that use these different methodologies. It also remains unclear exactly how the testosterone and estradiol levels present in amniotic fluid relate to those of the foetal circulation in human pregnancies.

    • Amniotic testosterone and psychological sex differences: A systematic review of the extreme male brain theory

      2020, Developmental Review
      Citation Excerpt :

      Table 1 displays the study characteristics. 4 reported ASD symptom scores (Auyeung et al., 2010, 2012; Auyeung, Baron-Cohen, Ashwin, Knickmeyer, Taylor & Hackett, 2009; Kung et al., 2016), 9 ASD-related cognition (Beking et al., 2018; Finegan et al., 1992; Grimshaw et al., 1995; Lutchmaya, Baron-Cohen, & Raggatt, 2001; Knickmeyer et al., 2005; Auyeung et al., 2006; Lust et al., 2010, 2011; Auyeung et al., 2012), 4 focused on social outcomes (Auyeung, 2006; Chapman et al., 2006; Knickmeyer, Baron-Cohen, Raggatt, Taylor, & Hackett, 2006; Lutchmaya, Baron-Cohen, & Raggatt, 2002), and 6 on behavioural and play-styles (Grimshaw, Sitarenios, & Finegan, 1995; Knickmeyer, Wheelright et al., 2005; Auyeung et al., 2009; Körner et al., 2019; Spencer et al., 2017; van de Beek, van Goozen, Buitelaar, & Cohen-Kettenis, 2009). 12 of these studies were conducted by Baron-Cohen’s group (Lutchmaya et al., 2001, 2002; Knickmeyer et al., 2005; Knickmeyer, Wheelright et al., 2005; Auyeung, 2006; Auyeung et al., 2009a, 2009b, 2010, 2012; Chapman et al., 2006; Knickmeyer et al., 2006).

    • Association between tendencies for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and the 2D:4D digit ratio: a cross-cultural replication in Germany and China

      2020, Early Human Development
      Citation Excerpt :

      Furthermore, evidence from a current longitudinal study by Beking and colleagues [10] shows that prenatal testosterone measured in amniotic fluid had an influence on brain lateralisation in boys but not in girls. Going beyond this, a recent study by Körner and colleagues [11] found that higher prenatal testosterone measured in amniotic fluid was associated with a weaker ability to delay gratification, more attention problems, and overactive behaviour in boys. Against this background, it might be hypothesised that higher prenatal testosterone levels in males compared to females may contribute to the skewed gender distribution in ADHD, such that males are twice to four times more often affected than females [1,12,13].

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text